zhabotynska@ling.ucsd.edu
4:00 p.m.
Humanities Bldg. 118
These and similar examples testify to the ubiquity of numbers which
we use daily without giving much thought to what number is. When this
question is posed, the answer (which at first seems to be easy) is
problematic. This problem is well known to philosophers who tried to
suggest a comprehensive explanation of the category of number, and who
found themselves in a deadlock. The "lock" seems to be not that "dead"
from the standpoint of cognitive science. Number is the product of the
human mind. As recent studies (Hurford 1987; Lakoff & Numez 2000)
show, in the creation of number the mind uses habitual elementary
ideas, or schemas. In my previous works (Zhabotynska 1992a, 1992b,
1992c, 1994 among others), I argue that such elementary schemas are
specifically arranged to yield a complex schema of number applied in
everyday life, mythology, and mathematics. In these domains, number
acquires individual properties that can be considered as the
elaboration and extension of one and the same complex schema.
In this presentation, I will center on the schema of Naive Number used
in everyday life. It is the concept of Naive Number that undergoes
transformations in mythology and mathematics, and it is this concept
which is used in natural language. The issue I intend to discuss is
word-formation where the concept of Number integrates with the other
concepts belonging to the categories of Thing, Quality,
Existence/Action, Mode, Location, Time, and Evaluation. The topics in
focus are the cognitive mechanisms of hypostasis (blending of the
part-of-speech categories), metonymy and metaphor. All these phenomena
relate to profiling a particular element of the Number's complex
schema. Integration of such an element with the other concepts is
regulated by the conceptual network. In my recent works (Zhabotynska
1999; 2001: to appear), I maintain that the foundation of this network
is construed by five basic frames. In the forthcoming talk, I will
attempt to demonstrate that these frames can be exposed in the
semantics of a considerable amount of the English data. Some data from
slang and cant are not for sensitive ears, which, I hope, will not
bewilder linguists.
Lakoff, G. & Nunez, R. 2000. Where Mathematics Comes From. New York:
Basic Books.
Zhabotynska, S.A. 1992a. Cognitive and Nominative Aspects of
the Numeral Class. Moscow: Institute of Linguistics, Russian Academy
of Sciences. [In Russian].
___________ 1992b. Knowledge representation structure in the
concept of number. In Language and Knowledge Representation
Structures. Moscow: Institute for Scientific Information in
Humanities, 123-148. [In Russian].
___________. 1992c. Number in everyday life, mythology and
mathematics: the coils of a semiotic spiral? In Language and
Culture. Papers of The 2nd International Conference. Kiev: Ukrainian
Institute for Foreign Relations, 69-77. [In Russian].
___________. 1994. "FOUR" as a cultural concept:
psychological foundations. In Language and Culture. Papers of the 3rd
International Conference. Kiev: Ukrainian Institute for Foreign
Relations, 60-67. [In Russian].
______________. 1999. Conceptual analysis: Types of Frames.
Messenger of Cherkasy State University. V. 11: Philology,
12-25. Cherkasy, Ukraine: Cherkasy University Press. [In Russian].
______________. 2001: to appear. A Conceptual Model of the
Part-of-Speech System (Germanic and Slavic Languages). Cherkasy,
Ukraine: Cherkasy University Press. [In Russian].
Abstract
Folks dream of having a 1 million dollar account in a number 1
bank. They are on cloud 9 when they live in a 5-star hotel (but not in
room 13!). They enjoy movies where Agent 007 demonstrates again and
again that he is a cat of 9 lives.
References
Hurford, J. R. 1987. Language and Number: The Emergence of a Cognitive
System: New York: Basil Blackwell.